Magic in the Biblical World: From the Rod of Aaron to the Ring of Solomon (Library Of New Testament Studies) Review

Magic in the Biblical World: From the Rod of Aaron to the Ring of Solomon (Library Of New Testament Studies)
Average Reviews:

(More customer reviews)
Are you looking to buy Magic in the Biblical World: From the Rod of Aaron to the Ring of Solomon (Library Of New Testament Studies)? Here is the right place to find the great deals. we can offer discounts of up to 90% on Magic in the Biblical World: From the Rod of Aaron to the Ring of Solomon (Library Of New Testament Studies). Check out the link below:

>> Click Here to See Compare Prices and Get the Best Offers

Magic in the Biblical World: From the Rod of Aaron to the Ring of Solomon (Library Of New Testament Studies) ReviewMagic in the Biblical World: From the Rod of Aaron to the Ring of Solomon edited by Todd Klutz (Journal for the Study of the New Testament Supplement Series: T. & T. Clark Publishers) (Hardcover) The category `magic', long used to signify an allegedly substantive type of activity distinguishable from `religion', has nearly been dismantled by recent theoretical developments in religious studies. While recognizing and at times reinforcing those developments, the essays in this collection show that there is still much to be learned about the cultural context of early Judaism and Christianity by analysing ancient sources which either use `magic' as a label for deviant religiosity or valorise behaviour of a broadly magicoreligious variety. Through sustained engagement with texts ranging from Exodus 7-9 and 18 to the Testament of Solomon and Sefer ha-Razim, this volume focuses on materials that challenge the familiar boundaries between miracle, magic and medicine; yet it also heightens awareness of the way unsuspecting use of a sick sign (e.g. `magic') can impede critical understanding of texts and their respective contexts of reception. Excerpt: Since several thorough treatments of the theoretical debates concerning magic have already been published by others, (For discussion that stays close to our interest in interpreting the biblical materials, see esp. Garrett, The Demise of the Devil, pp. 1-36; and S. Ricks, 'The Magician as Outsider in the Hebrew Bible and the New Testament', in Meyer and Mirecki (eds.), Ancient Magic and Ritual Power, pp. 131-44. For treatments that deal at length with relevant theories and debates in the history of the social sciences, see G. Cunningham, Religion and Magic: Approaches and Theories (New York: New York University Press, 1999); F. Bowie. The Anthropology of Religion (Oxfords Basil Blackwell, 2000), pp. 13-28; J. Skorupski, Symbol and Theory: A Philosophical Study of Theories of Religion in Social Anthropology (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1976); and Smith, 'Trading Places', esp. pp. 13-20.) and with various facets of the same topic being picked up in the studies that follow here, a comprehensive overview of the matter would be superfluous in this context. Nevertheless, a couple of issues closely related to this one-namely, whether the growing scholarly reticence to continue using words like 'magic' to denote the religious beliefs of others (i.e. non-Westerners and ancient peoples) is anything more than an academic fad, inspired less by serious commitment to correcting Western images of the two-thirds world as intellectually backward than by troubled liberal consciences trying to staunch a global hemorrhage with an anthropological Band-Aid; and, if indeed it is more than that, what exactly makes it deserve our attention at this juncture, especially since the high cost of misunderstanding the religion of others has been demonstrated afresh by the leading news stories of 2001 and 2002. (See, e.g., Amy Waldman, 'How in a Little British Town Jihad Found Young Converts' (http.//www.nytimes.com/2002/04/24/international/europe/24BRIT.htm1); and Andrew Sullivan. 'This is a Religious War', New York Times, 7 October 2001, section 6) Significantly, scholarly references to `magic' and 'the magical world view' have normally been accompanied by either explicit or implicit denigration of the mental capacities of people who traffic in such stuff. (J. Fitzmyer (Luke the Theologian: Aspects of His Teaching [London: Geoffrey Chapman, 1989], pp. 150-51) for instance, after describing Luke's demonic aetiology of illness as symptomatic of 'protological thinking', asserts that `ancient folk, unable to diagnose properly an illness or discern its secondary, natural causality, ascribed it to a preternatural being, a spirit or a demon').Thus, while theoretical resources for debunking this scholarly tendency are normally sought (and sometimes found) in the writings of social anthropologists, a critique with greater power and relevance might be derived from a discipline whose interests have more to do with human mind and cognition namely, linguistics, and especially the contributions made to this field by Noam Chomsky. If for instance Chomsky is right (as I believe he is) that the ongoing slaughter of some of the world's most impover¬ished people on the altar of Western affluence is legitimated chiefly through a web of ethnocentric fictions and self-flattering illusions, collectively nurtured and disseminated by Western governments and their corporate and media patrons, then Western academic discourse about `primitive peoples' and their `magical' or 'superstitious' mentalities fully deserves any suspi¬cions we might have about its sources and functions. (Although Chomsky himself, to my knowledge, nowhere addresses our particu¬lar concern explicitly, his discussion of academic inquiry into the relationship between human language, intellectual endowment, and race ('Equality: Language Development, Human Intelligence, and Social Organization', in J. Peck [ed.], The Chomsky Reader [New York: Pantheon Books, 1987]. pp. 195-202) has direct implications that are con¬sistent with the inference I am drawing here.)
This suspicion is only intensified, moreover, by the implications of Chomsky's revolutionary theories of human language and mind. To be more precise, as Chomsky and many influenced by him strongly argue that essentially comparable levels of grammatical complexity and communica¬tive competence are manifest in all the world's different systems of natural language, (See, e.g., N. Chomsky, Language and Mind (New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, enlarged edn, 1972), pp. 112-14; and S. Pinker, The Language Instinct. The New Science of Language and Mind (London: Penguin, 1994), pp. 25-31.) Chomskyan linguistic theory can easily be understood to cohere on a deep level with Chomskyan political analysis, the universalism of the former reinforcing the egalitarianism of the latter and thus dealing its own heavy blow to the use of words like `magical', `naïve' or `protological' in discussions of non-Western modes of human cognition. (On the subtle but profound unity of Chomsky's political and linguistic ideas, see J. Lyons, Chomsky (Modern Masters; London: Fontana/Collins, 1970), pp. 13-15.)
Notwithstanding the seriousness with which Chomsky's work deserves to be considered, however, and if the reader will permit use of a more autobiographical register for a couple of paragraphs, I myself should confess to being plagued by doubts nurtured by the late Ernest Gellner, who raised a number of very awkward questions specifically in regard to Chomsky's criticism of the American social scientists who assisted their government's war effort in Vietnam. (E. Gellner, Relativism and the Social Sciences (Cambridge: Cambridge Universitv Press. 1985), p. 101) To be more precise, and as Gellner himself observed, Chomsky emphatically denounces the morality and politics of these academics yet `cannot refrain, at the same time, from scorning their scientific pretensions'. 20 But what if, Gellner asks, the scien¬tific claims of Chomsky's academic opponents could be proved to be no mere pretence, but rather genuine and valid? Would America's political objectives and military strategies in Vietnam therefore have been morally less objectionable?' And more broadly, can we really expect valid science always to dovetail so conveniently with our noblest and kindest intuitions about what is moral and best for the flourishing of human beings in general? And to make explicit the connection between these questions and our present topic-is it really the case that our most valid science conclulively demonstrates the existence of something akin to universal equality in human cognitive competence, and with it the theoretical bankruptcy of intellectualist and similar traditions of writing about `magic', when this same science is by and large our science and therefore a product chiefly of particular traditions and institutions that distinguish the modem West (for better or worse) from other sociocultural formations?
Letting myself be reduced by this sort of dilemma to a state of ethical and political indecisiveness, I confess, must constitute some kind of uniquely awful (and largely Western) vice; but it is not one without a few redeem¬ing effects: at the very least, were Ito start my academic career afresh and find myself practising one variety or another of social science, the scholars I would join ranks with would clearly not be those pictured by Gellner as strutting confidently about, `shaking their paradigm like a coxcomb, instructing the students, advising authorities'. No, almost certainly, I would find my home instead among Gellner's `more becomingly doubt-ridden' family of theorists, and quickly learn how to grumble that no Mephistopheles from CNN or the British Foreign Office had offered to buy my soul. And thus, in some very fractional but not imperceptible way, the world would become a better and nicer place.
Accordingly, readers will find no strutting in this introduction, no confident pronouncements about how, at last, the long debate concerning `magic and religion' can be brought to a universally satisfying resolution. The farthest I can go in this direction is to say, right here and very briefly, that the linguistic distinction between competence and performance may well offer us a way out of this dilemma; for although Chomsky and his heirs are almost certainly right to assert that remarkable psycholinguistic and mental competencies are equally manifest in all the world's known societies and natural language systems, this fact scarcely guarantees that these competencies will be used in any given context to produce discourse in either an intelligent or a humane fashion, as the numerous examples adduced by Chomsky himself of ill-informed and inhumane discourse in Western political...Read more›Magic in the Biblical World: From the Rod of Aaron to the Ring of Solomon (Library Of New Testament Studies) Overview

Want to learn more information about Magic in the Biblical World: From the Rod of Aaron to the Ring of Solomon (Library Of New Testament Studies)?

>> Click Here to See All Customer Reviews & Ratings Now

0 comments:

Post a Comment